Master the Art of Intention: 10 Techniques for Better Results
Discover ten evidence based strategies to improve your ability to manifest intentions.
Surprising research suggests mind-matter interaction works backwards in time: Instead of mental intention pushing forward to influence random events, desired outcomes may pull backwards through time to arrange their causes. Mind over matter or retrocausation?
Audio companion to this article below.
In a 2006 article initially published in the Journal of Scientific Exploration, researcher Dean Radin presents evidence that at its core, challenges our conventional understanding of cause and effect. Through a series of carefully designed experiments testing mind-matter interaction (MMI), Radin's research suggests that human intention may influence random events through a retrocausal process rather than through direct forward causation.
Let that sink in for a moment.
There are two cognitive leaps that need to be made here. First, we have to accept the core premise that mind-matter interaction is even possible (think telekinesis), but then secondly, the effect seems to be retrocausal. In other words, for the reported experiments - instead of intention influencing a future event, it appears that the desired outcome is somehow pulling back through time to influence the earlier events that led to it!
For decades, researchers have studied whether human consciousness can influence random physical systems, particularly random number generators (RNGs). This field of study includes numerous experiments with consciousness-based effects, unexplained influences on random systems, and controversial evidence suggesting mind-matter interaction. While meta-analyses suggest these effects are real and independently replicable, the mechanism behind them has remained a complete mystery. Traditional explanations assume that consciousness somehow "pushes" on these systems in a forward-moving causal way, like mental telekinesis. However, Radin proposes that this model doesn't fit the experimental data well.
Here's the problem: If mind-matter interaction worked through straightforward forward causation, increasing the number of trials in an experiment should produce increasingly strong statistical evidence. With even a tiny consistent effect, running enough trials would eventually yield unambiguous proof. But after hundreds of experiments, this isn't what researchers observe. This suggests the basic assumption of forward causation may be wrong.
As an aside, I would argue that perhaps the basic assumption of mind-matter interaction might be wrong. There's been a ton of meta-analysis done across independent studies to support the hypothesis. However, there are known potential weaknesses with the method.
I'm not a statistician, but I am a big proponent of the idea that "extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence" and when something as fundamental as the arrow-of-time or cause-effect is being questioned, that requires a huge amount of supporting data.
To test different models of mind-matter interaction, Radin designed a clever experiment that featured random numbers traversing a Markov chain - a scheme where each random state influenced the probability of the next state. Participants were asked to try to use mental influence to produce a specific outcome (in this case a sound), but they didn't know the complex pathway of random decisions required to reach that outcome.
The image above details how a three stage Markov chain was implemented in the experiment. A random number generator was used to produce an initial state of either a 0 or 1, and then subsequent stages in the chain provided an opportunity for the participant to "nudge" via intention the subsequent random numbers in the direction of the desired outcome based on weighted probabilities.
This setup allowed researchers to examine whether (if any) mental influence showed up as:
The key was that these different possibilities would produce distinct patterns in how the probabilities shifted across the Markov chain of random events.
A clear pattern was apparent after running multiple experiments with different variations. Amazingly, the data matched what would be expected from a retrocausal effect - meaning the intended future outcome was somehow "pulling" the random output to make it happen. This was the only conclusion that made sense because the observed probability curves looked very different from what standard forward causation would predict.
The above diagram from the paper shows how after 100 trials, the output in Stage 3 deviated from the expected 50% probability. (See the paper for additional detail as to how the hypothesis for a retrocausal effect was supported.)
Particularly telling was the comparison between true random number generators and pseudo-random number generators (PRNGs). With forward causation, you'd expect stronger effects with true RNGs since there are more opportunities to influence the system. But the effects were statistically indistinguishable between the two types, suggesting the influence wasn't operating through direct manipulation of the random process itself.
If validated by further research, these findings would have profound implications for our understanding of consciousness, causality, and the nature of time itself. Some deep thoughts to consider:
Radin addresses several obvious objections in the paper:
First, wouldn't backwards causation create paradoxes? Not necessarily, if the future is inherently probabilistic rather than deterministic. Retrocausal effects may influence what becomes more or less likely to happen, without creating logical impossibilities.
Second, isn't this just selective reporting of data? The experiments were specifically designed to test different causal models, with clear predictions made in advance. The results consistently matched retrocausal patterns across multiple studies.
Third, couldn't this all be explained by ordinary statistical fluctuations? The effect sizes were modest but statistically significant, and most importantly, showed specific patterns matching theoretical predictions for retrocausation rather than random variation.
These results are undoubtably provocative and in need of further validation. However, surprisingly there is a growing body of evidence for retrocausal effects in other contexts, including:
The paper makes a compelling case that rather than dismissing the findings as impossible, we should seriously investigate whether our basic assumptions about causality and time should be reexamined.
There are many opportunities for future research in this area:
This research suggests we may need to fundamentally rethink our models of how consciousness interacts with physical reality.
The fact that intention might influence events through retrocausation rather than direct mental force is a radical idea, but it also potentially resolves many puzzling aspects of previous research in this area. It also aligns with modern physics' increasingly sophisticated understanding of time.
As with any cutting-edge research challenging fundamental assumptions, these findings will need extensive validation and replication. However, they also provide a potential new framework for investigating the mysteries of consciousness, causality, and the nature of time itself.
Dear reader, as you can see from the types of topics I cover, I do not write for an algorithm and I do NOT display ads of any kind. If you like this content, please consider becoming a subscriber to keep this site going! Also, if you enjoyed this article please consider sharing on social media! Thank you again for your support and readership.
Join as a Supporter to comment